The place and role of (moral) anger in organizational behavior studies
Genre
Journal ArticleDate
2016-07-01Author
Lindebaum, DGeddes, D
Subject
angeraggression
indignation
morality
moral anger
revenge
righteous anger
moral outrage
empathic anger
personal anger
whistle-blowing
injustice
conflict
emotion
Permanent link to this record
http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12613/5175
Metadata
Show full item recordDOI
10.1002/job.2065Abstract
© 2015 The Authors Journal of Organizational Behavior Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd. The aim of this article is to conceptually delineate moral anger from other related constructs. Drawing upon social functional accounts of anger, we contend that distilling the finer nuances of morally motivated anger and its expression can increase the precision with which we examine prosocial forms of anger (e.g., redressing injustice), in general, and moral anger, in particular. Without this differentiation, we assert that (i) moral anger remains theoretically elusive, (ii) that this thwarts our ability to methodologically capture the unique variance moral anger can explain in important work outcomes, and that (iii) this can promote ill-informed organizational policies and practice. We offer a four-factor definition of moral anger and demonstrate the utility of this characterization as a distinct construct with application for workplace phenomena such as, but not limited to, whistle-blowing. Next, we outline a future research agenda, including how to operationalize the construct and address issues of construct, discriminant, and convergent validity. Finally, we argue for greater appreciation of anger's prosocial functions and concomitant understanding that many anger displays can be justified and lack harmful intent. If allowed and addressed with interest and concern, these emotional displays can lead to improved organizational practice. © 2015 The Authors Journal of Organizational Behavior Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Citation to related work
WileyHas part
Journal of Organizational BehaviorADA compliance
For Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA) accommodation, including help with reading this content, please contact scholarshare@temple.eduae974a485f413a2113503eed53cd6c53
http://dx.doi.org/10.34944/dspace/5157
Scopus Count
Collections
Related items
Showing items related by title, author, creator and subject.
-
Therapeutic alliance and treatment outcome in cognitive-behavioral and supportive psychotherapy for intermittent explosive disorderMcCloskey, Michael S.; Fauber, Robert L.; Heimberg, Richard G.; Drabick, Deborah A.; Kendall, Philip C; O'Hayer, C. Virginia (Temple University. Libraries, 2021)Therapeutic alliance is widely considered one of the factors most associated with treatment success in psychotherapy across a variety of outcomes. However, these effects may differ based on treatment approach or who is rating alliance (client, therapist, or third-party observer). Notably, research on this relationship among individuals with primary aggression problems is limited, with no study to date investigating therapeutic alliance in psychotherapy among individuals with intermittent explosive disorder (IED), the only psychiatric disorder for which affective aggression is pathognomonic. The current study sought to fill this gap by exploring the role of therapeutic alliance on a range of outcomes among 51 adults with IED who participated in a randomized clinical trial comparing cognitive-behavioral and supportive psychotherapy. Therapeutic alliance was assessed by clients, therapists, and an observer at week four of treatment, and outcomes included time in treatment, anger, aggression, emotion dysregulation, and IED remission status. Results showed that alliance was positively associated with reduced anger and aggression at post-treatment. Although alliance was more highly rated in the cognitive-behavioral condition, the alliance-outcome relationship did not differ based on treatment condition. There were no differences found between raters of alliance. These findings support the importance of developing and maintaining a strong relationship in psychotherapy with individuals diagnosed with IED.
-
The Time Course of Anger: An Experimental InvestigationMcCloskey, Michael S.; Giovannetti, Tania; Drabick, Deborah A.; Fauber, Robert L.; Efran, Jay S.; Schaffer, Matthew (Temple University. Libraries, 2016)Conceptualizations of anger have suffered from a lack of research investigating the temporal dynamics of anger episodes. Furthermore, though some studies have provided valuable insights into the time course of anger, no study to date has utilized a standardized laboratory paradigm designed to mimic an interpersonal provocation. The purpose of this study was to characterize the time course of the affective, physiological, and behavioral components of anger in response to a standardized provocation. Our second aim was to assess potential effects of trait anger, trait aggression, trait hostility, and emotion regulation deficits on the time course of the different components of anger. Participants (n = 82) engaged in the Modified Taylor Aggression Paradigm (MTAP), a laboratory measure of anger/aggression in which provocation is manipulated by varying electric shocks selected for the participant by an (unbeknownst to the participant) fictitious opponent. This study utilized a modified version of the classic TAP that simulated an acute interpersonal provocation that one might encounter in the “real world.” Subjective anger, physiological arousal (as evidenced by heart rate [HR], galvanic skin response [GSR], and high-frequency heart rate variability [HF HRV]), and the behavioral expression of anger (aggression) were measured throughout the task before, during and after provocation. Consistent with previous research, results showed that the rise time to peak levels of most outcome variables was significantly faster than the return time from peak back to baseline. Additionally, results showed that the majority of the time course variables were not correlated with one another providing evidence for the idea that different components of anger have independent time courses. Contrary to our hypotheses, trait variables were largely unrelated to time course variables. The current study provides further evidence for the relationship between the rise time and return time in the time course of subjective, physiological and behavioral manifestations of anger using a standardized and ecologically valid provocation task.
-
A Model of Workplace Anger Response CyclesGeddes, Deanna; Deckop, John Raymond; Blau, Gary J.; Gibson, Donald E. (Temple University. Libraries, 2012)Anger is considered a basic emotion, evoked typically by something that interferes with the person's execution of plans or attainment of goals, as well as the perception of harm and unfair circumstances or behavior (Shaver, Schwartz, Kirson, & O'Connor, 1987). Research examining anger is largely focused on negative individual and organizational outcomes, such as raised blood pressure and heart disease, decreased productivity, reduced job satisfaction and increased job stress, and aggressive responses (Geddes & Callister, 2007; Gibson, Schweitzer, Callister, & Gray, 2009). However, the paradoxical nature of anger--having the capacity of both negative and positive consequences--is increasingly acknowledged by influential scholars (Geddes & Callister, 2007; Gibson & Callister, 2010; Miron-Spektor & Rafaeli, 2009). For instance, both theory and data point out that anger expression can have important adaptive functions such as signaling that goals are blocked, communicating to others that their actions are viewed as threatening, and mobilizing protective actions when dealing with a perceived threat (Averill, 1983; Izard, 1993; Tafrate, Kassinove, & Dundin, 2002). In addition, most research on consequences of anger expression in the workplace focuses on individuals' characteristics and the intensity of the expression of anger. This focus, however, doesn't capture the significant social components of emotions including how their expression shapes the emotions, thoughts and behaviors of others (Hareli & Rafaeli, 2008; Parkinson, Fischer, & Manstead, 2005). The goal of this dissertation is to advance understanding of the complexity of workplace anger expression, focusing specifically on outcomes and social components of emotions. To do this, the dissertation proposes a Model of Workplace Anger Response Cycles (WARC). The model shows that the outcomes of anger expression are influenced by the interaction of the expresser and a social partner and the context where this interaction occurs--e.g. organizational culture, emotional climate, and anger expression tolerance. In addition, the model proposes that these interactions form emotional cycles and identifies factors that affect the association of anger expression and its positive or negative outcomes, namely, intensity of the message, characteristics of the individuals involved and their relationship. Given the complexity and number of variables included in the WARC model, only some were studied empirically for this dissertation. The empirical study reported here involved collection of data from full and part time employees (n=177) through on-line surveys, which were analyzed using Partial Least Square Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM). The results show the salience of relationship quality, observer response, power, and gender in the explanation of outcomes of anger expression in the workplace. In particular, results support existing evidence that female expressions of anger are negatively judged compared to those by male (e.g., Brescoll & Uhlmann, 2008; Gibson, Schweitzer, Callister, & Gray, 2009; Lewis, 2000). Furthermore, findings indicate that even in organizations where anger expressions are more accepted, in general, expressions of anger by females are less welcome than those from males. Additionally, this study provides further evidence that when anger is expressed to a lower power individual, results are more positive--i.e., anger feelings are reduced and problematic situations improve--than when anger is expressed to a higher power individual (Fitness, 2000; Kuppens, Van Mechelen, & Meulders, 2004; Van Kleef & Côté, 2007). Finally, findings of this study show that relationship quality among the interactants is critical to explain the outcomes of anger expression. Furthermore, results show that relationship quality effects are partially mediated by observer response, and moderated by zone of anger tolerance. Thus, results confirm the underlying general hypothesis of this work, that emotional experiences and consequent related interactions are strongly tied to context characteristics. Therefore, to truly understand this type of phenomena and be able to predict results, consideration of multi-level variables must be integrated to the analysis.